What if a high end speaker measures really badly?


You know, it's true that I feel listening is more important than measurements and that it's generally difficult to really tie together measurements with pleasure.  Below 0.05% THD do I care?  No I do not.  I really don't care. The number tells me nothing about whether I'd like the amp more or not anymore.

In this one memorable review for the Alta Audio Adam speaker, I really felt shivers go up my spine when I looked at the measurements, especially at ~$20kUSD.   This looks like an absolute hot mess.  Does it sound this bad though?  I certainly don't have the $20K to test that out myself. What do you all think? 

erik_squires

Showing 6 responses by audiokinesis

Narrow-band peaks and dips which occur within about 1/3 octave of one another tend to be "averaged" by the ear. Therefore, that on-axis frequency response graph looks far worse to the eyes than it sounds to the ears.

The on-axis dip at 3 kHz is wide enough to survive the ear's "averaging" characteristic, BUT it corresponds with an off-axis rise in that region.  So my guess is the designer put a dip at the bottom end of the tweeter’s range to compensate for an off-axis "flare". Imo this is a good design choice.

The on-axis emphasis centered around 11 kHz can be compensated for by listening off-axis.

In fact, it looks to me like at about maybe 10-20 degrees off-axis the 3 kHz dip fills in and the 11 kHz bump smooths out.

My guess is that this speaker would sound excellent with proper set-up.

Duke

Not an Alta Audio dealer

@erik_squires wrote:  "If the peaks and dips are not audible, as @audiokinesis suggests then I wonder why and how they went through all the trouble to introduce them in the first place?"

I think the up/down peak/dip jogs are artifacts of the enclosure design, which is apparently a transmission line/port hybrid.  There are corresponding little spikes in the impedance curve that look like what I've seen in some of my transmission line builds. 

@wyoboy wrote: "@audiokinesis With your discussion of on-axis/off-axis, made me wonder if that means these speakers should not be toed-in--ie so listening position is off-axis? or perhaps toed-in to where the "crossing point" is in front of listener?"

I’ve seen a different Alta Audio model (which I enjoyed enormously) at an audio show and they had them toed-in just a little bit. Here’s a link to a YouTube video with the Adams, again toed-in just a little bit; conversation with the designer starts around 3:00. If there is anything amiss that correlates with the Stereophile on-axis curve, it’s apparently below my detection threshold:

 

They don’t strike me as designed for time-intensity trading; that is, axes criss-crossing in front of the listening position. So my instinct is that little or no toe-in would probably work best.

 

Not all dips are created equal even if they look similar. How the dip was caused makes a significant difference.

Comb filter effects tend to be considerably more benign than the dip in an anechoic or quasi-anechoic curve implies. This is because the energy is actually still there, and arrives after a reflection or two, and tends to perceptually fill in the dip. The ear/brain system does not process sound the same way that microphones do.

The floor bounce dip is a comb filter effect that looks dreadful on paper but is relatively benign because it is filled in by the subsequent room reflections. If you want to compare what the floor bounce dip sounds like with and without reflections, have someone talk to you from a few feet away indoors. Pay attention to the timbre of their voice. Then walk outdoors and have that person talk to you again from about the same distance, again paying attention to the timbre of their voice. The direct sound is the exact same; the reflections are what’s different. Without reflections filling in the floor-bounce dip, their voice sounds much thinner.

Another virtually ubiquitous comb filter effect is the 2 kHz stereo dip. At about 2 kHz the signal from the left speaker arrives at the right ear delayed by 1/2 wavelength (because of the path-length difference), and a comb-filter cancellation dip results. Likewise at the left ear the same thing happens. In approximately anechoic conditions (including near-field listening) this dip can be audible, and mixing engineers actually use this dip in their well-damped mixing rooms to find exactly where their ears should be when using nearfield monitors. But in a normal listening set-up, this comb filter effect is filled in by the in-room reflections and is not noticeable.

On the other hand, a dip that is due to reduced output from a driver is far more likely to be audible and objectionable because it shows up in the direct sound and in the reflections.

Microphone height can make a great deal of difference in the on-axis frequency response measurement depending on how directional the drivers are in the vertical plane and how their outputs are combining at the microphone location. In practice there is often considerably less change in timbre at the listening position than one might expect from the differing curves taken at different microphone heights because the in-room contribution of the reflections tends to average things out.

Don’t get me wrong, I am very much pro-measurements! The first 90 percent or so of my work on a design is purely measurement-based. But sometimes measurements taken at face value can be misleading; ime the actual cause of frequency response anomalies needs to be understood and taken into account. It takes a LOT of measurements to tell enough of the story to get a reliable picture of what’s going on, and even then the characteristics of the human hearing system can result in perception not precisely matching up with expectations.

Duke

@erik_squires wrote:  "I'm reading @audiokinesis but avoiding commenting when I just don't know what I'm talking about.  Honestly my experience with very poor measuring speakers is rather limited so I yield the floor. 🤣"

Here is Floyd Toole being much more succinct than me:

"A ripple in the on-axis curve may be acoustical interference (not very audible, or inaudible), but if it replicates in spatially averaged measurements it is almost certainly evidence of a resonance."

@helomech wrote:

"It’s quite apparent from his posts that Audiokineses doesn’t have a strong grasp of speaker science."

Could you elaborate?  I'd like to know where you disagree with me.  I'm not infallible. 

Thanks.